Family Surroundings

Family environment, peer relationships and loneliness, cognitive development, temperament (specifically behavioral inhibition), shyness, early attachment, and social skill deficits all are cited every bit factors that tin can contribute to social feet disorder.

From: Social Anxiety (Third Edition) , 2014

Assessment with Belatedly-Life Families: Problems and Instruments

Brian D. Carpenter , Elizabeth A. Mulligan , in Handbook of Cess in Clinical Gerontology (2nd Edition), 2010

Family unit Environment Calibration

The Family Environment Calibration (FES; Moos & Moos, 1994) was adult from an interactionist framework with the purpose of describing the social environment of the family unit. As such, although the constructs tapped by the FES overlap with some of those in other instruments, the underlying focus of the FES is on how family members relate to and behave with one another, all inside the larger social context. Families are assessed on 3 dimensions, within which there are a number of subscales: relationship (cohesion, expressiveness, and conflict subscales); personal growth (independence, accomplishment orientation, intellectual-cultural orientation, active-recreational orientation, and moral-religious accent); system maintenance (arrangement and control). In addition to the long form of the FES, other forms enable assessment of perceptions of the current (existent class), wished for (ideal form), and expected (expectations course) family. A brusk form also exists equally a subset of items from each subscale. An even briefer form, the Family unit Relationships Index (Fri), includes only three subscales (cohesiveness, expressiveness, and conflict).

The FES provides a comprehensive family cess that is quite different in terms of some aspects of its content than the other instruments in this section. For instance, its attention to interest in political and cultural engagement places the family squarely in a larger social context, which may be valuable information when working with late-life families. Still, use of the musical instrument with tardily-life families and diverse families has been express. Another disadvantage is the instrument'southward relative length, although users could isolate specific subscales of interest. Moreover, in recent years there has been some debate most the reliability of some subscales, mayhap highlighting the measurement error inherent in truthful/false items. This has been specially truthful in research with families from various indigenous and cultural backgrounds, eastward.g., Puerto Ricans, Vietnamese (Munet-Vilaro & Egan, 1990). In addition, although cistron analytic studies have consistently found 2–3 solutions, these factors take not usually mapped onto the three dimensions proposed by the scale'southward authors. Therefore, for researchers using the FES information technology is important to calculate and report reliability indices for each sample and to consider how unstable validity might bear on associations with other dependent and independent variables. Besides, clinicians may demand to keep in heed how depression reliability and validity might cloud estimation and prediction.

Read total chapter

URL:

https://www.sciencedirect.com/scientific discipline/commodity/pii/B9780123749611100119

Psychosocial Resources

Shelley Due east. Taylor , Joelle I. Broffman , in Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 2011

iii.1.1 Socioeconomic condition

In addition to the family environment, there are other aspects of the early on life environment that confer risk for poor psychosocial resources and long-term adverse mental and concrete health outcomes. Chief among these factors is low babyhood SES.

There are well-established socioeconomic and racial disparities in mental and concrete wellness outcomes, such that the higher one moves on the SES ladder, the lower 1'southward chance for psychological distress and for adverse health outcomes (Adler & Rehkopf, 2008). The relation is, for the well-nigh part, linear rather than asymptotic, which means that each step upwardly the ladder brings increased resistance to psychological distress, to disease, and to premature mortality. Psychosocial resources are a likely contributor to these disparities.

Early on childhood SES is believed to be a pivotal context for the development of psychosocial resources. Research indicates that depression childhood SES is tied to perceptions of little command, pessimism, and poor social support, factors that may link SES to poor wellness (Adler et al., 1999; Finkelstein, Kubzansky, Capitman, & Goodman, 2007; Gallo, de los Monteros, & Shivpuri, 2009; Repetti et al., 2002; Taylor & Seeman, 1999). For example, there is an SES gradient in cynicism (just not optimism) (Taylor & Seeman, 1999), suggesting that harsh early life experiences contribute to the development of enduring pessimistic expectations (Carver et al., 2010). Among low-SES individuals who do accept potent behavior in personal mastery, mental and physical health outcomes are equivalent to those seen in high-SES groups (Lachman & Weaver, 1998). To a lesser extent, self-esteem shows an SES gradient (Adler et al., 1999). Perceived social support demonstrates a stiff SES gradient, such that those of college SES in childhood and/or adulthood study greater social back up resources (Kessler et al., 1992). SES also links to coping style. In one study, exposure to uncontrollable stressors was associated with greater avoidant coping in impoverished women, which was, in turn, associated with an enhanced run a risk for depression (Rayburn et al., 2005). Lack of social support, which is distributed by SES, also can prompt avoidance oriented coping under stress (Manning, Catley, Harris, Mayo, & Ahluwalia, 2005).

Matthews and colleagues (Gallo, Bogart, Vranceanu, & Matthews, 2005; Gallo & Matthews, 2003) have proposed a Reserve Chapters Model, maintaining that psychosocial resources are significantly associated with SES level, such that the higher 1 is in SES, the greater one'due south "reserve capacity" to bargain with stressful events. In an empirical test of these ideas, women with varying levels of SES monitored their positive and negative psychosocial experiences and emotions across 2 days. Measures of psychosocial resource included perceived control, positive affect, and social strain. Low SES was associated with lower levels of these resources, and depression perceived command and social strain contributed to the clan between SES and well-beingness (Gallo et al., 2005, 2009; Matthews, Gallo, & Taylor, 2010). Resource appear to play a part of straight arbitration as opposed to moderation (Matthews et al., 2010). Indeed, the evidence for the importance of psychosocial resource every bit a mediator of the result of low SES on poor health is stronger than the bear witness suggesting that stress mediates this relation (Matthews et al., 2010).

Read full chapter

URL:

https://world wide web.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/B9780123855220000019

Family Relationships and Children's Stress Responses

Rachel K. Lucas-Thompson , Wendy A. Goldberg , in Advances in Child Development and Beliefs, 2011

C Mechanisms

How can stress levels in early on family unit environments affect health years later? Ane mechanism rests on the touch of agin family conditions on parenting. Economic stress has been linked to deteriorated quality of parenting that includes high conflict, negative and restrictive parenting styles, or chaotic and neglectful parenting ( Emery & Laumann-Billings, 1998; McLoyd, 1998). The influence of chronic exposure on both harsh parenting practices and cumulative family risks places children at run a risk for heightened stress responses (Larson, Russ, Crall, & Halfon, 2008) and emotional dysregulation (Repetti et al., 2002).

Also, the stressful weather condition of depression family income and low parental teaching tin mean that children practise not have admission to quality health care or good health habits. Children from depression-income families, for example, are more likely to accept exposure to higher levels of lead, an environmental blood toxin, which has been shown to mediate the association betwixt low family income and greater cortisol levels following laboratory stress tasks (Gump et al., 2007).

Additional of import mechanisms are negative emotionality (Lehman et al., 2005) and self-regulating abilities that link babyhood family relationships to long-term vulnerability to stress-related illnesses. Repetti et al. (2002) argue that harsh family environments compromise emotional and social functioning and in this fashion, negatively touch health outcomes. In contrast, loftier parental warmth tin buffer adolescents from the negative effects of stress on cortisol levels (Evans et al., 2007). Finally, there is the factor of genetic endowment. Shared genes among members of the same family unit may predispose parents to bear in ways that "are consistent with risky family environments and that make children more than reactive to stress" (Hanson & Chen, 2010, p. 400).

Read full chapter

URL:

https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/B9780123864918000074

Genes and Individual Differences

James R. Flynn , in Intelligence and Human Progress, 2013

five.2.iv What is the Planet's Name?

There is no existent alternative to citing family environment equally the main distorting influence only there is an ambivalence. There are iii components that affect the variance between the IQs of individuals: genes, family unit environment (frequently called mutual environs), and current environment (often called uncommon environment).

Genes would be merely a minor distortion and if anything, would cause a pocket-size underestimate of the pull of family unit environment. The notion that children far above the median for vocabulary should benefit from a greater and greater advantage for genes as they historic period is not plausible. But in that location might be a pocket-size negative effect. As the handicap they suffer from an unfavorable family surround fades, they might demand slightly less elevated genes to attain such a good vocabulary.

As nosotros have seen, current environs has a lot to practise with the bad or practiced luck no ane tin protect people from. No doubt, those that score high on vocabulary at any age enjoy better luck than the average person. Only the question is whether this reward would ascension or fall with historic period. If so, it would mean something like: high performers have a sure balance in terms of favor of good over bad teachers at 6; loftier performers would run across that balance change for better or worse in terms of fewer or more teenage traumas at 16; and loftier performers would come across it modify over again in terms of more than rather than less unemployment at 26. If this were true, you would look that twin or kinship studies would show that the pct (of IQ variance) current environs explains would modify from age to age. In fact, kinship studies show that uncommon environment is steady at about 25% at all ages (Haworth et al., 2010; Jensen, 1998).

Read full chapter

URL:

https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/commodity/pii/B978012417014800005X

ICD-eleven—Comparison With DSM-5 and Implications for Child & Adolescent Psychiatric Disorders

1000.Due east. Garralda , in Positive Mental Health, Fighting Stigma and Promoting Resiliency for Children and Adolescents, 2016

The Importance of Generic Psychosocial Influences for Child and Adolescent Mental Health

The undeniable role of dysfunctional psychosocial, family, and school environments for the development and/or maintenance of child psychiatric disorders has strongly influenced how bug have been viewed and managed in CAMHS and the use of generic over disorder-specific treatments. Some children, for example, are presented with difficulties involving stress in parent–kid relationships without necessarily beingness linked to mental health bug in the child (Carrey & Gregson, 2008). Expertise in CAMHS has often focused on techniques to reduce stress, convey therapeutic empathy and agreement to the child, and improve parenting techniques and family relationships. These techniques in expert hands can be beneficial for children with any blazon and level of difficulty, in the same way that antipyretics are helpful for many types of febrile affliction, or advice and expert help to engage in a good for you lifestyle for many different types of concrete disorders. Some of these trans-diagnostic techniques will remain of import and helpful.

Nevertheless, diagnosis of private disorders as described in ICD and DSM has been primal to empirical evaluations of both psychotherapeutic and medical treatments for child and boyish mental health problems. Specific disorder-based treatments include certain kinds of family therapy for anorexia nervosa, parenting work for acquit disorders, cognitive behavioral therapies for mood disorders, specific medications for ADHD, schizophrenia, or other psychotic states, and for anxiety and depressive disorders. Increasingly specialist diagnostic and treatment CAMHS are being developed for disorders such every bit autism, ADHD, mood disorders, and obsessive compulsive disorder, and pediatric liaison services for children with joint medical and psychiatric problems. Bodies such as Overnice (National Found for Health and Intendance Excellence) in the United kingdom of great britain and northern ireland—which aims to aid practitioners deliver the best possible care for concrete and mental disorders, to provide the most effective treatments based on the almost up-to-engagement evidence, and provide value for money—accept published guidelines on a number of diagnosis-based child psychiatric disorders (https://world wide web.nice.org.uk).

Withal, it is well-established that in kid psychiatric exercise, specific techniques will oft demand to be complemented by trans-diagnostic interventions equally mentioned higher up.

Read full chapter

URL:

https://www.sciencedirect.com/scientific discipline/article/pii/B9780128043943000024

Beyond Emerge's Missing Marble

Kristin Hansen Lagattuta , ... Sarah Tashjian , in Advances in Child Development and Beliefs, 2015

iii.2 Parent–Kid Interactions

Some other highly researched area on individual differences in ToM concerns how young children's family surroundings may contribute to their emerging psychological understanding. In particular, the quality of children'due south attachment to caregivers and the frequency that children engage in causal-explanatory talk most mental states and emotions accept been implicated as important factors. Several studies take found concurrent as well every bit longitudinal associations betwixt attachment security and ToM in 2- to 6-twelvemonth-olds ( de Rosnay & Harris, 2002; Fonagy, Redfern, & Charman, 1997; Laible & Thompson, 1998; Meins, Fernyhough, Russell, & Clark-Carter, 1998; Repacholi & Trapolini, 2004; Steele, Steele, Croft, & Fonagy, 1999). Longitudinal and cross-sectional studies have farther documented significant relations between parent–child soapbox almost mental states and preschoolers' reasoning about behavior and emotions (see Hughes, White, & Ensor, 2014 for a review). At that place is also mounting evidence that zipper security and parent–kid discourse interconnect: More secure dyads not but have more than frequent mentalistic conversations, but parent–kid discourse quality mediates the relation between attachment and ToM (Mcquaid, Bigelow, McLaughlin, & MacLean, 2008; Meins et al., 2002; Ontai & Thompson, 2008; Raikes & Thompson, 2006).

The question remains whether these same variables continue to predict more avant-garde ToM in middle childhood and beyond. Researchers examining relations betwixt mental land language and ToM in half dozen- to 10-year-olds accept found no pregnant association between children's production of mental land terms and their operation on second-gild false-belief tasks (Charman & Shmueli-Goetz, 1998; Longobardi, Spataro, & Renna, 2014; Meins, Fernyhough, Johnson, & Lidstone, 2006). Relations continue to exist, however, betwixt older children'due south comprehension of the meaning of different mental country terms (metalinguistic noesis) and their performance on advanced ToM (Grazzani & Ornaghi, 2012; Longobardi et al., 2014); perhaps non surprising because metalinguistic awareness is a straight measure of ToM. These data indicate that parent–kid conversations about mental states may be more critical for early on conquering of ToM, but less of import once certain ToM levels take been mastered. Future enquiry is needed to make a more definitive conclusion.

Research on parent–kid attachment in centre babyhood has primarily focused on how attachment security predicts peer human relationship quality and mental health (Kerns & Brumariu, 2014; Kerns & Richardson, 2005) rather than whether it predicts children's reasoning most mental states and emotions. Yet, mental models of attachment relationships are presumed to incorporate thoughts, emotions, and beliefs about the cocky and others, stemming from early on experiences with caregivers (Bowlby, 1973), a very ToM-like construct. Whereas attachment quality may predict children'due south ability to pass tasks measuring conventionalities and emotion noesis in early childhood (as discussed previously), in centre childhood and later in life, zipper quality may instead predict nuances in ToM. That is, children and adults may employ zipper relationships, whether secure or insecure, equally a heuristic for predicting and explaining social behaviors and relationships. In our enquiry, for instance, we are investigating whether attachment security predicts how 4- to 12-twelvemonth-olds and adults differentially weight social bear witness (i.eastward., focus more on negative versus positive actions) to brand future-oriented judgments most mental states and emotions. Potentially, older children and adults with insecure attachment may exhibit more pronounced negativity biases than securely attached individuals.

Read full chapter

URL:

https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0065240714000330

Nature and Nurture Effects On Children's Outcomes

Bruce Sacerdote , in Handbook of Social Economics, 2011

III Canonical Results from the Behavioral Genetics Literature

As noted earlier, the most voluminous and heavily cited part of the BG literature measures the contributions of genes and family environs to IQ. There are numerous summaries of this IQ literature including Goldberger [1977], Bouchard and McGue [1981], Devlin et al. [1994], Jencks et al. [1972], and Taylor [1980]. Table I shows the mean of the estimated correlations in each of these meta-studies along with the number of individual studies incorporated.

Table I. Correlations in IQ between siblings, adoptive siblings, and identical twins

Meta report authors Number of studies considered Correlation for siblings raised together (non-adoptive, non identical twins) Correlation for adoptive sibs Correlation for identical twins Correlation for fraternal twins
Devlin, Daniels and Roeder (1994) 212 0.44 0.85
Bouchard and McGue (1981) 69 0.45 0.29 0.85
Golberger (1977) 7 0.51 0.31 0.91
Jencks et al. (1972) 18 0.54 0.42 0.86 0.58

The tabular array reports results from 4 surveys of the IQ literature and incorporate hundreds of private studies of twin and adoptee samples.

Data for Jencks et al. are as summarized by Taylor [1980] p. 46.

Devlin, Daniels, and Roeder reviewed 212 dissimilar studies of the IQ of twins. The mean correlation in IQ for studies of pairs of identical twins was .85. The correlations for congenial twins were similar to correlations for other siblings and averaged .44. 1 tin see immediately that in a simple model this volition generate a high estimated heritability; if 1 assumes that identical twins are twice as genetically related as other full siblings are and have twice the correlation in outcomes, equations (5) and (six) would lead one to conclude that all of the explained variance is genetic. Goldberger (1977) and Jencks et al. [1972] each reviewed a number of twin studies of IQ. The studies they review yielded like results. In the case of Jencks [1972], the correlation in IQ for identical twins is .86 versus .54 for other siblings.

Bouchard and McGue [1981] examined a large number of twin studies and adoption studies. The adoption studies find meaning correlation in IQ between adoptive siblings. The median correlation in IQ for adoptive siblings is .29 while the correlation for biological siblings raised together is .45. However, many of these studies are for adoptees less than age 18. Studies of older adoptive and biological siblings have institute that the correlation in IQ amid adoptees tends to fall significantly in adulthood while the correlation for biological siblings grows. Plomin et al. [1997, 2001].

Table Ii translates these sibling correlations into the behavioral genetics decomposition of variance in IQ into portions attributable to variance in genes, family (common) surround and divide environment. The twin designs find that a high proportion of explained variance in IQ is due to genes, and very piffling is due to family surroundings. Averaging over more than 200 studies, Devlin et al. testify the average finding is that 49% of the variance is genetic and 5% is attributable to family (common) environment. The Bouchard and McGue summary of correlations for twins finds like results, namely that 54% of variation is genetic and 4% is due to family environs. Non-shared environment (what economists would telephone call the remainder or unexplained variance) accounts for a substantial 40–50% of the variation in IQ.

Table II. IQ Results: Implied variance decomposition from the behavioral genetics model

Meta Studies Variance attributable to additive genetic furnishings Variance owing to non-additive genetic effects Full genetic Variance attributable to common environment Non-shared surroundings
Devlin, Daniels and Roeder (1994) 0.34 0.15 0.49 0.05 0.46
Golberger (1977) 0.47 0.xi 0.58 0.22 0.xx
Bouchard and McGue (1981) MZ vs DZ Twins* 0.54 0.04 0.42
Bouchard and McGue (1981) Adoptees* 0.32 0.29 0.39
Private Studies
Cherny and Cardon (1994) (For 9 year old Adoptees and Sibs) 0.60 0.xvi 0.24
*
Bouchard and McGue practice non calculate estimates of heritability from the sibling correlations they aggregate. Loehlin (1989) does this calculation using the Bouchard and McGue aggregates does not split ecology effects into common (family) and non-shared. I calculated these using the simple version of the BG model in equations (4) and (5).

The adoption studies find a larger proportion of variance in IQ owing to family environment. Cardon and Cherny'due south [1994] examination of nine-year-olds in the Colorado Adoption Projection found that 16% of the variation in IQ is attributable to family unit environment, and 60% is due to genes. The Bouchard and McGue summary of IQ correlations for adoptees implies that 29% of the variation is due family surroundings and 32% is due to genes. Averaging over the studies in Goldberger'southward [1977] literature summary, which includes both twin and adoption correlations, I find that 22% of the variation in IQ is due to family environment and 58% is due to genetic effects.

At that place is a disconnect betwixt the twin and adoption literatures with regard to the importance of family environment. One way to resolve this contradiction is to appeal to the findings that family surroundings furnishings on adoptees are profoundly attenuated in adulthood and that heritability rises with age (Pedersen et al. [1992] and McClearn et al. [1997]). However, another reasonable caption is that applying the simple version of the behavioral genetics model to pairs of identical and fraternal twins volition overstate heritability if identical twins face environments more similar than that faced for other siblings (Feldman and Otto [1997].) 6 Or, identical twins might bear upon each other'due south surround more than exercise congenial twins. Recall from Section Two that any factors which make outcomes for identical twins more similar than outcomes for fraternal twins are assigned to genetic furnishings. The supposition of the structural model is that sibling pairs raised in the aforementioned household take the same correlation in family or common environment. 1 could imagine that parents and teachers would exist fifty-fifty more likely to expect or demand similar operation from siblings who are identical twins. Parents may be more likely to provide similar ecology experiences for identical twins. In decomposing sources of earnings variation, Björkland Jäntii and Solon [2005] observe that allowing dissimilar types of sibling pairs to have unlike amounts of correlation in family unit surround greatly lowers the estimated heritability and raises the estimated impacts from family environment.

In Table III, I summarize the existing behavioral genetics studies of variance in years of education. In that location are far fewer BG studies of pedagogy and earnings than of IQ, and the nearly widely known studies are those washed past economists and sociologists. Behrman and Taubman [1989] use data on twins and their relatives from the National Academy of Science/National Research Quango sample. They compute years of schooling correlations for sixteen different pairs of relatives and fit the parameters of their model to match the predicted correlations with the sample correlations. Consistent with twin studies of IQ that find loftier heritability, Behrman and Taubman find that genetic effects explain 88% of the variation in schooling. vii Family environment explains little or none of the variance in schooling. Scarr and Weinberg [1994] examine adoptees and find that family surroundings explains xiii% of the variation. Withal, this study is based on only 59 adoptive sibling pairs. Teasdale and Owen [1984] have 163 pairs of adoptees and notice that variance family environment explains 5% of the variation in schooling.

Table Three. Years of didactics: Unsaid variance decomposition from the behavioral genetics model

Authors and sample Variance attributable to condiment genetic effects Variance attributable to non-additive genetic effects Total genetic Variance attributable to mutual environment Not-shared surroundings
Behrman and Taubman (1989) 2,000 twins pairs and their relatives NAS-NRC sample 0.88 (.002) −0.01 (.047) 0.88
Scarr and Weinberg (1994) 59 adoptive sibling pairs and 105 nonadoptive sibling pairs 0.38 0.13 0.49
Teasdale and Owen (1984) 163 pairs of adoptees from Danish National Register 0.678 0.678 0.052 0.270
Behrman, Taubman, and Wales (1977) 2,478 MZ and DZ Twins in the NAS-NRC sample 0.36 0.41 0.23

Scarr and Weinberg (1994) report adoptive and biological sibling correlations. I used equations (4) and (v) to translate this into the decomposition implied by the simplest grade of the BG model. Teasdale and Owen written report their results in variance of years of education explained by additive genes, common environment and dissever environs. I calculated the fractions explained past each factor. The NAS-NRC sample is a National Academy of Science - National Enquiry Council survey of twins performed in 1974.

Overall, to the extent that behavioral geneticists take performed nature-nurture decompositions using years of schooling as the outcome, the findings have mirrored the findings of the much larger IQ literature. Genetic effects play a large role, while in that location is only a small function for family unit environs. That statement is tempered a fleck by the Behrman, Taubman, and Wales study, and Scarr and Weinberg study, though that study had merely 59 pairs of adoptive siblings. A unlike but equally valid interpretation of the results in Table III would exist to say genetic effects conspicuously affair a corking deal in determining schooling, but that the portion attributable to family unit environment changes significantly depending on how one specifies the structural model.

In Table 4, I switch the outcome of interest to earnings and I written report results from two dissimilar studies. Björkland, Jäntti, and Solon [2005] used a large sample of siblings, twins and adoptees from the Statistics Sweden and Swedish Twin Registry. They derive formulae for the predicted correlations among nine different sibling types. They use weighted to the lowest degree squares to choose parameters to fit all-time the sample correlations to the predicted correlations from the models. One of the cardinal results from this report is that it matters a great bargain whether or not one constrains all sibling types reared together to have the same caste of correlation in family unit (common) surroundings. With such a constraint (Model ane), genes explicate 28% of the variance in earnings and family environment explains four%. 8 Past adding three additional parameters to allow for differing correlations in family surroundings amidst sibling pairs (Model 4), the importance of family unit (common) environment rises to xvi.four% and the genetic effects fall to nineteen.9%.

Table Four. Earnings: Implied variance decomposition from the behavioral genetics model

Authors and sample Variance owing genetic furnishings Variance attributable to common surround Variance attributable to non-shared environment
Björklund, Jäntti and Solon (2005) Model 1 Swedish Brothers Including Raised Apart, Together, Twins, Adoptees, Half Sibs .281 (.080) 0.038 (0.037) 0.681
Björklund, Jäntti and Solon (2005) Model 1 Swedish Sisters Including Raised Autonomously, Together, Twins, Adoptees, Half Sibs .245 (.080) 0.009 (0.037) 0.746
Björklund, Jäntti and Solon (2005) Model iv Swedish Brothers Including Raised Autonomously, Together, Twins, Adoptees, One-half Sibs 0.199 (0.157) 0.164 (0.158) 0.637
Behrman, Taubman, and Wales (1975) 0.45 0.13 −0.42

Björklund, Jäntti and Solon estimates the BG parameters to fit the ix sibling correlations in the data from nine sibling types (MZ raised together, MZ autonomously, DZ together, DZ apart, Total sibs together, full sibs apart, one-half sibs together, one-half sibs autonomously, adoptive sibs). The divergence between models 1 and four is that model 4 adds parameters to allow for unlike degrees of ecology correlation among different types of sibling pairs.

Table V shows the results from Loehlin's [2005] summary of the behavioral genetics literature on the determinants of personality traits. Like the IQ research, this is a rich literature and Loehlin considers hundreds of studies. He reports average correlations between parents and children for the well-nigh commonly measured aspects of personality, namely extraversion, agreeableness, conscientiousness, neuroticism, and openness. With regard to the determinants of personality traits, the literature has reached even more of a consensus than with regard to IQ. The first cavalcade is for the correlations betwixt parents and children when their biological parents heighten children. Correlations range from .11 to .17. When we consider adoptees and adoptive parents in cavalcade 2, the correlations almost disappear, falling to an average of .036. Cavalcade 3 reports correlations in traits for adoptees and their biological parents. Here the correlations rise nigh to the levels seen in cavalcade (1), that is, for the children raised past their biological parents. This evidence (which once more is a summary of hundreds of studies) is striking and certainly points strongly in the direction of genes being an important determinant of personality traits.

Tabular array V. Behavioral genetics results on personality traits meta report of correlations between parents and children

Parent child relationship
Biological and social Social, not biological Biological, not social
Dimension
Extraversion 0.14
(117, .010)
0.03
(40, .011)
0.16
(fifteen, .019)
Agreeableness 0.eleven
(65, .013)
0.01
(xvi, .021)
0.14
(3, .067)
Conscientiousness 0.09
(64, .013)
0.02
(26, .012)
0.xi
(2, .110)
Neuroticism 0.13
(131, .010)
0.05
(xl, .011)
0.11
(21, .022)
Openness 0.17
(24, .028)
0.07
(12, .031)
0.14
(1 - )

This is a summary of the literature on personality traits and is reprinted exactly from Loehlin (2005) Tabular array six.iii. Number of correlations that were averaged and the implied standard errors are in parentheses.

More recently, economists and other social scientists have begun to gauge the heritability of parameters that are primal to economic models of human behavior. For example, Cesarini et al. [2009] and Cesarini et al. [forthcoming] utilize twins data to estimate the heritability of preferences for risk taking and for fairness. In both cases the authors find substantial genetic influences and only a small role for shared environment.

As a final event of interest, I graph in Figure I some of the information from the Grilo and Pogue-Geile [1991] meta written report of correlations in weight, height and body mass index among total siblings raised together, adoptive siblings, and twins. Adoptive siblings accept nearly no correlation in body mass index. Full siblings raised together have a correlation of nigh .32. Interestingly fraternal twins show similar levels of correlation to other sibling pairs. The correlation in BMI jumps to .72 for identical twins. 9

Figure I. Correlations in Body Mass Alphabetize For Four Types of Sibling Pairs

Data are from meta-study done by Grilo and Pogue-Geile [1991]. Numbers for adoptive siblings add results from Sacerdote [2007] since Grilo and Pogue Geile have only one study with BMI figures. All calculations include same and mixed gender pairs.

Read full chapter

URL:

https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/commodity/pii/B9780444531872000012

Healthcare

S. Russ , ... N. Halfon , in Encyclopedia of Infant and Early Babyhood Development, 2008

Life course models

Evidence is accumulating from longitudinal accomplice studies that early childhood health indicators, such as birthweight, and family and social environments are strongly linked with later health and mental health outcomes. Starting with the pioneering work of David Barker on associations between birthweight and mid-life cardiovascular illness, the concept of developmental origins of adult disease (the and then-called Barker hypothesis) has gained credence. There is now also growing evidence that events and experiences in early on childhood can have a profound effect on later adult health. For case, exposure to corruption and family dysfunction in childhood has been associated with higher prevalence and severity of adult disease and mental disease. These findings underscore the need for healthcare in babyhood to consider carefully family and environmental influences on a young child's development, and the importance of optimizing wellness beyond all its dimensions in the early years for hereafter well-being and longevity.

Read total chapter

URL:

https://www.sciencedirect.com/scientific discipline/article/pii/B9780123708779000761

Alcohol, Sexual Risk Taking, and Sexually Transmitted Infections

Melissa A. Lewis , ... Dana Litt , in Principles of Addiction, 2013

Social Influences

Several commonly acknowledged social influences on the clan betwixt booze use and sexual behavior include the function of peers, family environment, modeling, and perceived social norms. These influences are thought to begin early in life and change beyond the life class. During adolescence, the function of peers becomes increasingly important as teenagers spend more time together, develop more than intimate relationships, and begin experimenting in romantic relationships. In addition, the time between boyhood and young adulthood (sometimes referred to as emerging adulthood taking place between 18 and 25 years of age) is characterized as a flow of exploration. Substance use peaks in the early twenties, and the majority of young adults (62% of males and 70% of females) are sexually agile before age 18.

Read full affiliate

URL:

https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/B9780123983367000656

Anxiety Disorder in Children

C.L. Donovan , S.H. Spence , in International Encyclopedia of the Social & Behavioral Sciences, 2001

2.iii Parenting Characteristics

The stiff family unit links constitute in childhood feet could as well be explained to some degree past parental beliefs and the family unit environments in which the children are brought up. Parenting behavior has been suggested to bear upon upon child feet in a number of means. From a learning theory perspective, certain forms of parenting behavior may increase the probability that children learn to respond in an broken-hearted manner and fail to acquire the skills needed to cope with the inevitable stressful events that occur during children'south lives. Observational studies have demonstrated that parents of anxious children are more than likely to model, prompt, and reinforce anxious behavior, such as avoidance and distress in stressful situations. Furthermore, parents of anxious children are more likely to draw their children's attention to the threatening aspects of situations and less likely to encourage 'brave' solutions (Rapee in press).

The parents of anxious children are also more likely to engage in behaviors that brand it less likely that children will learn how to solve stressful problems themselves. Empirical enquiry has found that parents of broken-hearted children demonstrate higher levels of overcontrolling and overprotective behaviors that disrupt coping skills development. As a grouping, they are also more likely to be critical of their child's coping attempts, thereby reducing children'south conviction in their abilities to solve their ain life bug (Dumas, La Freniere and Serketich 1995, Krohne and Hock 1991). These parenting styles may interact with babyhood temperament in explaining why some behaviorally inhibited children develop feet issues and some do not. For example, parental overprotection and overcontrol appears to exist influential in determining the stability of behavioral inhibition in children (Hirshfeld et al. 1997a, 1997b). Parental beliefs has also been establish to be important in determining the impact of traumatic life events upon childhood psychopathology. Post-obit trauma, children are more probable to develop emotional and behavioral difficulties if their parents react in an overprotective manner after the event (eastward.g., McFarlane 1987).

It is also important to consider that children have an influence upon parents, and anxious child behavior may cause parents to comport in item ways. In much of the literature to date, information technology is non articulate whether the overprotective behaviors of parents are definitely a cause of babyhood anxiety or whether they could be a consequence of living with an anxious kid. Future research needs to clarify these relationships.

Read full affiliate

URL:

https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/commodity/pii/B0080430767013504